Evolution of the female labour force participation rate in Canada, 1976-1994: a cohort analysis
Paul Beaudry, CIRANO and UBC
and
Thomas Lemieux, CIRANO and Université de Montréal
February 1998
We thank Richard Archambault and Louis Grignon for their comments
on a preliminary draft of this paper.
ABSTRACT
This paper assesses the contribution of cohort effects, age composition
effects, and macroeconomic factors in the evolution of the female labour
force participation rate in Canada between 1976 and 1994. Using data from
the Survey of Consumer Finances, we find that cohort effects are the main
factor behind the recent stagnation in female participation rates. Though
the poor macroeconomic performance of the Canadian economy during the 1990s
has also contributed to this phenomena, it cannot explain in itself why
the behaviour of female participation rates in the 1990s was so different
than in previous decades. We reach similar conclusions when we analyse
the evolution of the employment rate.
One related finding is that both the level and the slope of the age-participation
profiles of women have changed over time. While older cohorts had profiles
that were sloping up between the ages of 25 and 50, younger cohorts exhibit
much flatter (and higher) profiles for the same age range. In other words,
age-participation profiles of women increasing look like those of men which
are flat at very high levels before declining after age 50.
RÉSUMÉ
Dans ce texte, nous évaluons le rôle des effets de cohortes, de la
structure d'âge et des facteurs macro-économiques dans l'évolution du
taux d'activité des femmes au Canada entre 1976 et 1994. Le résultat
principal qui ressort de notre analyse des données de l'enquête des finances
des consommateurs est que les effets de cohortes sont le facteur clef permettant
d'expliquer le plafonnement récent de ces taux d'activité. Bien que la
performance macro-économique peu enviable de l'économie canadienne dans
les années quatre-vingt-dix ait elle aussi contribué à ce phénomène
de plafonnement, il n'en reste pas moins que seuls les effets de cohortes
parviennent à expliquer pourquoi les taux d'activité ont crû beaucoup
plus rapidement dans les années soixante-dix et quatre-vingt que dans
les années quatre-vingt-dix. Nous tirons les mêmes conclusions lorsque
nous analysons les taux d'emploi plutôt que les taux d'activité.
Il ressort aussi de notre étude qu'à la fois le niveau et la pente
des profils de participation en fonction de l'âge (profils d'âge) ont
changé à travers le temps. Alors que la pente des profils d'âge était
positive entre l'âge de 25 et 50 ans pour les cohortes entrées depuis
longtemps sur le marché du travail, ces profils sont beaucoup plus plats
pour celle entrées plus récemment sur le marché. En d'autres termes,
les profils d'âge des femmes ressemblent de plus en plus à ceux des hommes
qui sont, eux aussi, plutôt plats jusqu'à l'âge de 50 ans.
Introduction
This paper studies the behavior of labour force participation among
women between the ages of 25 and 64 for the years 1976 to 1994. The goal
of this study is to determine whether the stagnation, and in some cases
decline, in the female labour force participation rate in the 1990s is
a temporary phenomenon tied to the poor growth performance of the economy
or whether it is an ongoing effect, which may signal that the process of
integration of women into the work force is almost complete. A cohort analysis
is used to examine this issue.
The methodology involves isolating the effect of three separate factors
on the participation rate of women. To this end, we follow cohorts of women
over time, that is, we track the participation rate of representative groups
of women who entered into the work force at a given point in time (e.g.
women who were 25 years old in 1976). We then decompose a cohort's participation
rate into, first, a macroeconomic effect that by definition is common
across cohorts. Recession and structural phenomena such as the generosity
of the employment insurance system are some of the factors that may cause
a macroeconomic effect. The second factor is the age or life
cycle effect, which shows how the cohort's participation rate changes
as the cohort ages. The third factor is the cohort specific effect,
which shows differences between cohorts for a given age and macroeconomic
effect. For example, if the cohort that entered the labour force in 1976
has a participation rate that is 10 percent higher than that of the cohort
that entered the labour force in 1966 at the same age and under similar
macroeconomic conditions, the 1976 cohort is said to exhibit a 10 percent
cohort effect relative to the 1966 cohort.
Our results indicate that cohort effects are likely the dominant factor
in explaining the recent stagnation in female labour force participation
rates. The same result was obtained when the labour force participation
rate was replaced by the employment rate. Cohort effects help explain both
the large increase in participation and employment rates during the 1970s
and 1980s, as well as their stagnation in the 1990s. The 1989-1994 recession
merely amplified the stagnation phenomenon; it also explains the observed
decline in a number of demographic groups. These results show, however,
that stagnation would have occurred even had more favourable macroeconomic
conditions prevailed.
This paper is divided into five sections. Section 1 outlines the data
used and provides some illustrative graphs. Section 2 presents the actual
cohort analyses. In Section 3, the results of those analyses are used to
break down the evolution of the labour force participation rate into macroeconomic,
age and cohort effects. Section 4 examines result robustness, studying
both the influence of changes in the population's education level on changes
in the participation and employment rates, and the effect of the generosity
of the employment insurance program, a major structural macroeconomic factor.
And finally, Section 5 makes some projections for future participation
and employment rates. Participation and employment rates will be analysed
simultaneously to ascertain that the evolution of the participation rate
does not merely reflect changes in the way Canadian women "classify" themselves
in the labour market.
Section 1: Data and descriptive statistics
The data used were obtained from the Survey of Consumer Finances (SCF)
for the years 1976, 1978, 1980, 1982, 1983, 1985, 1987, 1989, 1991, 1993
and 1994 (survey years). These years were used because: 1) from 1976 to
1982, the survey was taken only every other year; 2) since then, the survey
has been taken every year except 1984; and 3) 1994 was the last year available
when we began this study. The years represented (about every other year)
provide a fairly coherent sample across time.
Individuals were grouped into two-year cohorts according to their date
of permanent entry into the labour force. This was defined, somewhat arbitrarily,
as the even-numbered year in which the woman in question was 25 or 26 years
old (e.g. a woman born in 1941 is in the "entering" cohort for 1966). Thus
for each even year, all women between 25 and 64 are divided into 20 cohorts
(25-26, 27-28, ..., 63-64). In total, 29 cohorts entered into the labour
force between 1936 and 1992.
Note that Statistics Canada's public use files of the SCF from before
1982 provide data only for heads of households and spouses; we therefore
confined our analysis to this sub-sample for the entire 1976-1994 period.
Labour force activity (employment, unemployment or non-participation) is
determined based on individual responses to the usual LFS questions (for
the month of April in the SCF). The evolution of the labour force participation
rate, represented by a solid line, and the employment rate, represented
by a dotted line, for each cohort is shown in Figure 1 (age is indicated
in each graph). All the information used in this study is presented in
a raw form in this figure. Those cohorts that entered the labour force
first are shown only in the early years, while those that entered last
appear only in later years. Only the "middle" cohorts (those that entered
the labour force between 1954 and 1972) are shown in all years.
The figure shows that the evolution of the labour force participation
and employment rates is similar for all cohorts. Both these rates tend
to increase from the age of 25 to 45-50 years, then decrease rapidly until
age 65. Participation and employment rates are obviously higher for those
cohorts that entered the labour force most recently than for the others.
Aggregate data (ages 25-64, 25-44 and 45-64) are presented in Figure
2. The figure reproduce for our SCF sample the trends that we are trying
to explain, i.e. the stagnation, and in some cases decline, in participation
and employment rates for all age brackets but the oldest (45-64 years)
in the female population.
Other descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1, which illustrates
age composition and education levels (percentage of women with a high school
education or less) for each of the years studied. The table shows quite
a young population during the period from 1976 to 1994. About 65 percent
of women between the ages of 25 and 64 during these years were 44 or younger.
The impact of the baby boom/baby bust on the population's age composition
is also clearly visible. This helps explain the increase in the proportion
of women aged 35-44 since the beginning of the 1980s; the first wave of
boomers born in 1946 reached the age of 35 in 1981. The same phenomenon
occurred at the beginning of the 1990s as the first of the boomers reached
45. It is now the baby bust generation, those women born after 1965, who
make up the 25-34 year-old segment.
The statistics presented in Table 1 also show a steady increase in level
of education: the percentage of women with a high school education or less
dropped from 73.6 percent in 1976 to 54.3 percent in 1994. This trend,
however, is slightly exaggerated by the revamping of the questions on education
in the LFS in 1990.
Section 2: Cohort Analysis
2a. Econometric Model
An econometric model is used to examine the separate roles played by
the macroeconomic, cohort and age effects on labour force participation
and employment rates. The dependent variable used in the regressions is
the participation (or employment) rate pjt for cohort
j at time t expressed in "log-odds" form ln(pjt/(l-pjt)).
For example, p74,84 represents the labour force participation
rate for the cohort that entered the labour force in 1974 (j = 74)
during the year 1984 (t = 84). This functional form is used to account
for the special nature of variable pjt, whose value is
always between 0 and 1. It ensures that the predicted value will always
be between 0 and 1, which would not be the case if a standard linear specification
were used instead.
In most of the estimated models, only one macroeconomic variable is
used, the unemployment rate among men aged 25 to 44. Although certain long-term
trends in this rate may be determined by structural factors, it is clear
that its short-term fluctuations are mainly reflective of the evolution
of the economic climate. Other variables such as the output gap may be
used in addition to unemployment rate, but we prefer to concentrate on
the latter, because of its simplicity; however, the results must be interpreted
with caution. The scope of the macroeconomic effect will, however, be broadened
in Section 4 by adding other variables.
For the age effect, a fourth degree polynomial that allows variations
in participation rate over the entire life cycle is used. A flexible functional
form is also used to show the cohort effect. A third degree polynomial
yields the equation:
(1) ln(pjt/(l-pjt)) = α
+ δurt + β1j
+ β2j2 + β3j3
+ γ1ajt + γ2ajt2
+ γ3ajt3
+ γ4ajt4
where urt represents the unemployment rate among men between
25 and 44.
One characteristic of equation (1) is that the age profile for each
cohort, i.e. the evolution of the labour force participation rate over
the life cycle, is similar for each cohort; they differ only in terms of
the intercept. In other words, the model allows a vertical displacement
of the life cycle profile from one cohort to another while forcing the
shape of the profile, and thus the slope, to be identical for each cohort.
A more general model is produced by introducing age-cohort interaction
terms to allow the age effect to vary from one cohort to another. This
was done with the following model, which incorporates an age-cohort (ajtj)
and an age-cohort squared interaction term:
(2) ln(pjt/(1-pjt)) = α
+δurt +β1j
+β2j2 +γ1ajt
+γ2ajt2
+γ3ajt3
+ γ4ajt4
+θ1ajtj +θ2ajtj2
If second or higher order polynomial terms are omitted, equation (2)
shows that the age effect on ln(pjt/(1-pjt)) is equal
to γ1+θ1j.
If θ1 is positive, the age
effect will be greater for those cohorts which most recently entered the
labour force (highest j) than for the others, and vice versa. Coefficient
θ1 thus allows the life cycle
profile to vary from one cohort to another.
Graph 1 illustrates the advantages of equation (2) over equation (1),
which does not include the age-cohort interaction term. Without such interaction
terms, the intercept is the only difference between different cohorts’
age profiles (Graph 1a). The same increase in participation at career outset
and the same decrease in participation at career end is shown for every
cohort. The age profile is clearly more flexible in Graph 1b where interaction
terms are introduced. In this graph, the "new" cohort has both a higher
ordinate value at the origin and a shallower slope. This results in a higher
and more stable age profile at career outset than in the previous cohorts
(the "old" cohorts on the graph). This profile is also more similar to
that for men, whose participation rates are fairly high and stable until
the age of about fifty. The situation shown in Graph 1b is therefore more
consistent with the idea of a convergence between men’s and women’s
labour force participation rates, or increasing participation of women
in the labour force, than that shown in Graph 1a.
In Graph 1b, the cohort effect is concentrated at career outset, participation
rates before the age of 40 for the new cohort being much higher than those
for the old cohort, while the rates are reasonably comparable after the
age of 50. The impact of the entry of the new cohort on the aggregate labour
participation rate would thus be felt most strongly during the first 10
or 20 years after the cohort’s arrival, while in Graph 1a, its influence
is shown as continuing throughout the life cycle. In other words, the entry
of new cohorts in Graph 1b should result in a rapid increase in the aggregate
participation rate, followed by a period of stagnation. Graph 1a, on the
other hand, shows a constant increase in the aggregate participation rate.
Graph 2 illustrates the impact of the arrival of new cohorts in the
two cases discussed above, with those cohorts entering the labour force
after 1970 considered "new" cohorts and those entering before 1970 considered
"old" cohorts. The graph clearly shows that only the presence of an age-cohort
interaction effect explains the stagnation phenomenon.
An often-mentioned problem with cohort analyses is the impossibility
of separately identifying cohort effects, year effects (macroeconomic effects),
and age effects because of the linear dependence between them. In fact,
since ajt=25+t-j, the three variables (ajt,
j and t) are perfectly collinear. This study proceeds on
the implicit assumption that variable urt captures any systematic
macroeconomic effect and that there is no other temporal trend in this
effect. That said, econometric models (such as (1) and (2)) can never explain
all the variations in the data (R squared is less than 1). As a rule,
a residual macroeconomic effect is obtained, representing the macroeconomic
variation in the data that cannot be explained by other variables in the
model. If during a period, say the nineties, we were to find a large residual
we would interpret this as indicating that participation in this period
has experienced a macroeconomic effect not captured by its standard comovement
with the unemployment rate.
2b. Results
Equations (1) and (2) were estimated using weighted ordinary least squares,
with cohort size j at time t used as the weights. The results
are shown in Table 2 for employment rates (columns 1 to 3) and participation
rates (columns 4 to 6). For model (1), note that all the coefficients are
significant except for the rate of unemployment among men 25 to 44 (columns
1 and 4). That effect becomes significant, however, when age-cohort interaction
terms from model (2) are introduced (columns 2 and 5). Also note that interaction
term coefficients are highly significant, and that R squared for model
(2) is higher than for model (1).
We also present the results of the regressions when the sample is limited
to the 1976-1989 period (columns 3 and 6). The purpose of this exercise
is to assess whether the levelling off of participation and employment
rates in the 1990s was predictable from the behaviour of these rates prior
to 1990. The results indicate that the estimated parameters for 1976-1989
are very similar to those for the period as a whole. We shall return to
the question of the stagnation of the participation and employment rates.
To facilitate presentation of the results, it is simpler to use a graphical
approach than to examine the numbers presented in Table 2 in detail. For
each rate (participation and employment) and each model (1 and 2), we present
the following four graphs: Graph (a) shows the cohort effect at age 44
— i.e. the variations in the participation and employment rates attributable
to the cohort effect at a precise point in the life cycle. Graph (b) shows
the age effect throughout the life cycle for a typical cohort (the one
which entered the labour force in 1964). Graph (c) presents a similar result
for six cohorts (the ones which entered the labour force in 1940, 1950,
1960, 1970, 1980 and 1990) to illustrate cohort differences over the entire
age profile. It should be noted however that Graph (b) shows a predicted
age profile for the entire life cycle while Graph (c) shows the profile
only for the ages at which the cohort in question is observed in the data
(1976 to 1994). Finally, Graph (d) indicates the degree to which the estimated
model for 1976-1989 can be used to predict participation and employment
rates for the entire 1976-1994 period.
Rather than discussing each of the graphs in detail, we will confine
ourselves to noting a few highlights:
• All the estimated models indicate a levelling off in cohort effects
(figures 3a to 6a); the participation and employment rates for the 1992
cohort are comparable to the ones for the cohorts which entered the labour
force in the 1980s (or at least, the cohorts will all be comparable once
they have reached age 44).
• The participation and employment rates peak around age 50 (figures
3b to 6b).
• The younger cohorts have flatter age profiles (shallower slopes)
early in their careers. This pattern is particularly pronounced for model
2, which includes age-cohort interaction (figures 4c and 6c).
• The model without interactions (model 1) provides no explanation
whatsoever of the stagnant participation rate (Figure 3d) and falling employment
rate (Figure 5d) observed in the 1990s. On the other hand, these phenomena
can readily be predicted from the model with interactions (model 2) estimated
for 1976-1989.
This last finding is the most interesting since it suggests that there
is nothing abnormal in the behaviour of the participation and employment
rates during the 1990s if we take into account cohort and age effects and
general macroeconomic conditions in Canada during this period (the unemployment
rate for men aged 25-44). Figures A1 and A2 show that comparable results
are obtained when equation 2 is estimated in levels (pjt as
a dependent variable) rather than in log-odds form.
Our results seem to indicate that in addition to unfavourable macroeconomic
conditions, the levelling off of cohort effects also contributed to the
trend observed in the 1990s. This hypothesis will be examined in detail
in the following section.
Section 3: Decomposition
We shall now perform a more formal analysis of the role of different
factors in the recent evolution of aggregate participation and employment
rates for all women aged 25-64 by decomposing this evolution into four
components: the macroeconomic effect related to the unemployment rate among
men aged 25-44 (the economic cycle), the residual macroeconomic effect,
the age effect and cohort effects. In terms of equation (2), it is relatively
easy to identify the first two factors, which correspond to the term δurt
and to the residuals of this equation.
More precisely, we first calculate the participation (or employment)
rate for each year, taking the weighted average of pjt values
for each t. The observed rate (pjt) is then replaced by the
predicted rate jt from the estimated model. The average of the
jt values for each t therefore represents the aggregate rate
predicted by the model. The difference between the observed aggregate rate
and the predicted rate represents the residual macroeconomic effect.
We then recalculate the prediction by replacing the observed unemployment
rate by the average of the unemployment rates over the entire sample (8.2
%). The difference between this new prediction and the preceding prediction
represents the macroeconomic effect related to the male unemployment rate,
which we also call the cyclical effect.
The cyclical and residual effects obtained in this manner are presented
in Figure 7a for the participation rate and 8a for the employment rate.
During the 1990s, the cyclical effect is in the order of -1% for the participation
rate and -2% for the employment rate. In other words, the female employment
rate would have been 2% higher in the 1990s if the male unemployment rate
had held steady at 8.2%.
Age and cohort effects are somewhat more complicated to understand because
of the interaction terms in equation (2). It should be noted, first of
all, that the age effect comes into play only to the extent that the population's
age composition changes over time. For example, the arrival of the baby
boomers in the labour force in the early 1970s considerably rejuvenated
the 25-64 year-old population as a whole. As these young women had below-average
participation rates, it should have been expected that this change in composition
would have had a negative effect on the aggregate participation rate and
vice versa.
It might therefore be supposed that to identify the age effect, it is
enough to recalculate the predicted rate using a uniform age composition
(5% of the population aged 25-64 in each 2-year age group) instead of the
observed age composition. The problem with this procedure is that it depends
on the cohorts present in the labour force in each year, since the age
profile is dependent on the cohorts through the interaction terms. This
procedure therefore serves to isolate the age effect plus the crossed age-cohort
effect.
The same problem arises when we want to isolate the role of cohorts.
For example, we can try to recalculate the predicted rates by replacing
the cohort effect expressed as β1j
+β2j2 +θ1ajtj
+θ2ajtj2
by the cohort effect obtained if the cohort is set at an arbitrary level
such as j=70 (β170 +β2702
+θ1ajt70 +θ2ajt702).
This gives us the cohort effect plus the crossed age-cohort effect, in
the same way as in the case of age. Once we have all this information,
however, it is possible to calculate the "pure" age effect (for a given
cohort composition) and the joint age-cohort effect separately.
These different effects are illustrated in figures 7b, 7c and 7d for
the participation rate and figures 8b, 8c and 8d for the employment rate.
Let us take the example of Figure 7b: in this case, we use the cohort which
entered the labour force in 1970 as a reference cohort for the decomposition.
The cohort effect thus indicates the difference between the observed rates
and the rates which would have prevailed had all the cohorts followed the
same age profile as cohort 17, other factors being kept constant. This
cohort effect is therefore the "pure" effect mentioned earlier. The graph
also shows the "pure" age effect (for a given cohort and other factors)
as well as the combined age and age-cohort effect (the age effect for the
observed cohorts in each year).
While it can be rather difficult to grasp all the details of these decompositions,
the results speak for themselves: it is really the cohort effect that dominates
the evolution of the participation and employment rates throughout the
1976-1994 period. The results are very similar regardless of which cohort
is used as a reference for the decompositions (1970, 1980 or 1990). We
find that cohort effects account for an increase of about 20 percentage
points in participation and employment rates between 1976 and 1994. At
the same time, the graphs clearly indicate that this phenomenon seems to
be coming to an end. By comparison, age effects play a relatively small
role in recent changes.
To sum up, our results indicate that the stagnation of female participation
and employment rates is primarily a structural phenomenon related to the
stabilization of the cohort effects which were responsible for the remarkable
increase in these rates in the 1970s and 1980s. The unfavourable macroeconomic
situation amplified this phenomenon but was not the root cause. The relative
performance of the participation and employment rates during the 1981-1983
and 1989-1994 recessions clearly illustrates this phenomenon; in 1981-1983,
the downward pressure on the rates from the macroeconomic effect was offset
by the cohort effects, pushing the rates up by one percentage point per
year, whereas in 1989-1994, due to the stabilization of cohort effects,
macroeconomic effects comparable to those of 1981-1983 resulted in lower
participation and employment rates.
To clarify the role of cohort effects, we illustrate their magnitude
at age 24, 34, 44, 54 and 64 in figures 9 and 10. Let us take for example
Figure 9c, which shows the cohort effect at age 44 by year of entry into
the labour force. The vertical line indicates the cohort which was aged
44 in 1994. The curve to the left of the line describes the evolution of
cohort effects during the 1976-1994 period. The curve to the right shows
the predicted evolution for the coming years (see Section 5 for more details).
The results indicate a general slowing trend for most of the ages under
consideration, attributable to cohort effects. This is particularly true
for the younger groups (ages 24 and 34), which explains why the levelling
off and declining trend is more pronounced for the 25-44 age bracket than
for the 45-64 bracket (figures 2a and 2b).
Section 4: Robustness analysis
4a. Education
We re-estimated the models separately for women who have pursued post-secondary
studies and those who have only a high school diploma or less. The highlights
of the results presented in figures A3, A4, A5 and A6 are:
• The decline in the participation and employment rates in the 1990s
is more pronounced among poorly educated women (figures A3d and A4d) than
for the female population as a whole. The growth in employment and participation
between 1976 and 1989 is also weaker within each education group than for
the population as a whole. A significant portion of the rise in the rates
for the population as a whole therefore seems to be attributable to the
increase in average education levels.
• Similarly, the cohort effects exhibit a decline for the most recent
cohorts in most cases (see in particular figures A4a and A6a). This result
suggests that the average quality of cohorts is declining, since a high
education level is a less selective characteristic than it was in the past.
• The younger cohorts have very high and very flat age profiles for
women who pursued post-secondary studies. These profiles are very similar
to the ones for men with the same education levels.
4b. Employment insurance
In Table 3, we present the results of the regressions when the employment
insurance subsidy rate is also used as a macroeconomic variable. The results
are not very conclusive, since the effect on the employment rate is negative
when we also control for the unemployment rate among men aged 25-44 (column
2). The decline in the subsidy rate during the 1980s should therefore have
increased the employment rate instead of lowering it. The effect on the
participation rate is not significant (column 4). This being said, including
the subsidy rate as a macroeconomic variable has little impact on the model's
other coefficients. Our conclusions about the role of cohort effects versus
macroeconomic effects during the 1990s therefore remain unchanged.
Section 5: Predictions
We will now attempt to predict the future evolution of the participation
and employment rates under two different macroeconomic scenarios: a 8.2%
unemployment rate for men aged 25-44 (the average over the 1976-1989 period)
and a 6.6% unemployment rate for the same group (the 1989 level). To do
so, we must make some assumptions about the cohorts which will enter the
labour force after 1994. To simplify the exercise, we will simply hypothesize
that the size and age profile of these cohorts will be similar to those
of the last cohort observed (the one which entered the labour force in
1992).
The results of the simulations are presented in figures 11 (participation
rate) and 12 (employment rate). The conclusions are the same in both cases:
large increases in the participation and employment rates are clearly a
thing of the past; in the future, these rates can be expected to hold relatively
stable. However, there is still room for a 2-3 percentage point increase
in the rates if the macroeconomic situation continues to improve. It is
illusory, though, to think that the rates could rise 5-10 percentage points
during the next period of expansion as they did over the 1983-1989 period.
The cohort effects which prevailed at the time are simply no longer present
today.
Conclusion
This study's main finding is that the levelling off of female participation
and employment rates is primarily a structural phenomenon related to the
stabilization of the cohort effects which accounted for the remarkable
increase in these rates in the 1970s and 1980s. The unfavourable macroeconomic
situation has amplified this phenomenon but is not the root cause. The
relative performance of the participation and employment rates during the
1981-1983 and 1989-1994 recessions clearly illustrates this phenomenon;
in 1981-1983, the downward pressure on the rates from the macroeconomic
effect was offset by the cohort effects, pushing the rates up by one percentage
point per year, whereas in 1989-1994, due to the stabilization of cohort
effects, macroeconomic effects comparable to those of 1981-1983 resulted
in lower participation and employment rates.
This result is strongly dependent on the amount of flexibility used
to capture cohort effects. It is essential that the age profile as a whole,
and particularly its slope, be allowed to vary from one cohort to another.
This makes it possible to accurately trace both the rise and the flattening
of the employment and participation profiles by age. These phenomena are
consistent with a convergence in the behaviour of men and women in the
labour market: men exhibit very high and very flat (at least until age
55) employment and participation profiles over their life cycle. The profiles
of recent female cohorts are therefore closer to those of men than to those
of older female cohorts.
Finally, the recent evolution of participation and employment rates
in the U.S. seems to corroborate our findings: a levelling off of the employment
and participation rates has been observed there as well, despite more favourable
macroeconomic conditions in the U.S. than in Canada since 1992. There too,
the structural phenomenon of cohort effects seems to be the dominant factor
in the evolution of female employment and participation rates since the
1970s.
REFERENCES
Beaudry, Paul, and David Green, "Cohort Patterns in Canadian Earnings:
Assessing the Role of Skill Premia in Inequality Trends," National Bureau
of Economic Research Working Paper No. 6132, August 1997.
Card, David, and W. Craig Riddell, "A Comparative Analysis of Unemployment
in Canada and the United States," in D. Card et R. Freeman (eds.) Small
Differences that Matter: Labor Markets and Income Maintenance in Canada
and the United States, Chicago: University of Chicago Press for NBER,
1993, pp. 149-189
Table 1: Descriptive statistics
Year
|
Age distribution in %
|
% with high school or less
|
Number of obs.
|
25-34
|
35-44
|
45-54
|
55-64
|
.
|
(1)
|
(2)
|
(3)
|
(4)
|
(5)
|
(6)
|
1976
|
40.9
|
24
|
19.8
|
15.3
|
73.6
|
12269
|
1978
|
39.1
|
24.9
|
20.5
|
15.5
|
75
|
17372
|
1980
|
39.9
|
26.1
|
19.1
|
14.9
|
74.7
|
18212
|
1982
|
39.5
|
26.2
|
19.1
|
15.3
|
73.5
|
18881
|
1983
|
39.2
|
27.5
|
18.9
|
14.4
|
72.7
|
19775
|
1985
|
38.7
|
29.3
|
17.7
|
14.3
|
71
|
19664
|
1987
|
37.9
|
29.9
|
18
|
14.3
|
70.4
|
17949
|
1989
|
37.1
|
31
|
18.3
|
13.6
|
68.3
|
21117
|
1991
|
34.9
|
32.3
|
19.3
|
13.6
|
59.7
|
26033
|
1993
|
31.4
|
32.8
|
21.6
|
14.2
|
56.2
|
22592
|
1994
|
32.8
|
32.7
|
21.2
|
13.3
|
54.3
|
22420
|
Total
|
37
|
29.2
|
19.4
|
14.3
|
67.2
|
216284
|
Table 2: Detailed results
of regressions
(standard deviation in brackets)
. |
Employment rate
|
Participation rate
|
1976-1994 |
1976-1994 |
1976-1989 |
1976-1994 |
1976-1994 |
1976-1989 |
(1)
|
(2)
|
(3)
|
(4)
|
(5)
|
(6)
|
Constant
|
0.792
(0.143)
|
-3.102
(0.291)
|
-2.912
-0.344)
|
0.997
(0.158)
|
-3.398
(0.245)
|
-2.746
(0.177)
|
Unemploy.
rate
|
-0.019
(0.052)
|
-0.029
(0.004)
|
-0.031
(0.003)
|
-0.004
(0.011)
|
-0.016
(0.003)
|
-0.014
(0.002)
|
Cohort
effect:a
|
. |
co
|
-1.83 (0.154) |
3.804 (0.383) |
3.506 (0.465) |
-2.227 (0.151) |
4.162 (0.319) |
3.267 (0.215) |
co^2
|
1.609 (0.131) |
-0.792 (0.118) |
-0.681 (0.15) |
1.892 (0.102) |
-0.851 (0.1) |
-0.561 (0.062) |
co^3
|
-0.289 (0.025) |
. |
. |
-0.336 (0.019) |
. |
. |
Age effect:
|
. |
Age/10
|
0.389
(0.053)
|
2.706
(0.175)
|
2.78
(0.216)
|
0.405
(0.051)
|
3.004
(0.124)
|
2.749
(0.126)
|
(Age/10)^2
|
-0.445
(0.024)
|
-1.742
(0.101)
|
-0.676
(0.027)
|
-0.471
(0.021)
|
-0.76
(0.032)
|
-0.701
(0.019)
|
(Age/10)^3
|
-0.13
(0.007)
|
-0.151
(0.007)
|
-0.161
(0.009)
|
-0.139
(0.006)
|
-0.162
(0.006)
|
-0.167
(0.009)
|
(Age/10)^4
|
0.031
(0.006)
|
0.032
(0.006)
|
0.024
(0.004)
|
0.032
(0.005)
|
0.034
(0.005)
|
0.027
(0.005)
|
Interactions:
|
. |
Age/10
*co
|
. |
-1.742
(0.101)
|
-1.902
(0.14)
|
|
-1.963
(0.067
|
-1.886
(0.091)
|
(Age/10)
*co^2
|
|
0.226
(0.021)
|
0.298
(0.021)
|
|
0.258
(0.016
|
0.305
(0.021)
|
R squared:
|
0.941
|
0.954
|
0.951
|
0.953
|
0.964
|
0.962
|
Number of observations
|
224
|
224
|
164
|
224
|
224
|
164
|
Table 3: Unemployment insurance effect
(Standard deviation in brackets)
. |
Employment rate
|
Participation rate
|
(1)
|
(2)
|
(3)
|
(4)
|
Constant
|
-2.654
(0.501)
|
-2.984
(0.294)
|
-3.157
(0.361)
|
-3.337
(0.249)
|
Unemploy.
rate |
|
-0.023
(0.006)
|
|
-0.012
(0.006)
|
UI subsidy
rate
|
-0.117
(0.03)
|
-0.074
(0.037)
|
-0.062
(0.029)
|
-0.038
(0.04)
|
Cohort effect: |
. |
co
|
3.486
(0.682)
|
3.858
(0.401)
|
3.987
(0.51)
|
4.189
(0.35)
|
co^2
|
-0.778
(0.209)
|
-0.841
(0.128)
|
-0.841
(0.159)
|
-0.876
(0.116)
|
Age effect: |
. |
Age/10
|
2.546
(0.331)
|
2.733
(0.181)
|
2.916
(0.232)
|
3.018
(0.135)
|
(Age/10)^2
|
-0.7
(0.063)
|
-0.715
(0.046)
|
-0.757
(0.049)
|
-0.766
(0.039)
|
(Age/10)^3
|
-0.151
(0.007)
|
-0.151
(0.007)
|
-0.162
(0.006)
|
-0.163
(0.006)
|
(Age/10)^4
|
0.032
(0.006)
|
0.032
(0.006)
|
0.033
(0.005)
|
0.033
(0.005)
|
Interactions: |
. |
Age/10
*co |
-1.727
(0.197)
|
-1.791
(0.109)
|
3.987
(0.51)
|
-1.988
(0.084)
|
(Age/10)
*co^2 |
0.226
(0.021)
|
0.226
(0.021)
|
-0.842
(0.159)
|
0.258
(0.016)
|
Rsquared |
0.952
|
0.955
|
0.964
|
0.964
|
Number of observations |
224
|
224
|
224
|
224
|
|